Evolutionary Psychology 101: Men’s Long Term Mating Preferences
Note: Much of this is cut and pasted from the original source. I do not have the time to rewrite all of it. I trimmed out quite a lot here, leaving the most salient points. Perhaps I will re-write it later in the future. No more of these lessons in evolutionary psychology for a while or maybe ever, these take a long time.
Introduction: What do Men Stand to Gain from Long Term Relationships?
Why would men want
to commit to a relationship at all? It is partly due to women’s preferences.
Women want to find a mate who is likely to commit.
Men who are willing to promise long-term
resources, protection, and investment in children are appealing to women, so
men who are willing to commit to the long term have a wider range of women from
which to choose. Willing men attract desirable women because women typically
desire lasting commitment, and highly desirable women are in the best position
to get what they want.
A third potential benefit is an increase
in the odds that the man is the father of the children a woman bears. Through
marriage, a man gains repeated sexual access—in many cases, exclusive sexual
access. Without recurrent or exclusive access, his certainty in paternity would
be jeopardized.
A fourth potential benefit of marriage
would have been an increase in the survival of the man’s children. In human
ancestral environments, it is likely that infants and young children more frequently
died without the prolonged investment from two parents or related kin (Hill
& Hurtado, 1996). Among the Ache Indians of Paraguay, for example, children
without an investing father suffer a death rate more than 10 percent higher
than children whose fathers remain alive.
Fathers have also had a strong hand in
arranging marriages for their sons and daughters.
Marriage also benefits men by an increase
in status. A man also gains potential allies through his wife’s family and
friends.
In summary the benefits for a man are:
(1)
increased odds of succeeding
in attracting a (wo)man (sic), (2) increased ability to attract a more
desirable mate, (3) increased paternity certainty, (4) increased survival of
his children, (5) increased reproductive success of children accrued through
paternal investment, (6) increased social status, (7) added coalitional allies,
(8) access to his partner’s resources and status, and (9) increased lifespan.
The problem of cryptic ovulation
in human females
Men need to be able to determine which
women are fertile and bear many children.
In chimpanzees the female does this through something called estrus, it
is an outward display of fertility. It is advertised by bright red swollen
genitals and scents towards the males.
Woman’s ovulation is cryptic. Sexual
activity for humans occurs throughout the female’s ovulation cycle.
So, for men the shift became from when
they were ovulating to which women were capable of bearing children. They
needed to determine a woman’s reproductive value, meaning how many children she
could have. A younger woman generally has more potential to have kids than an
older one on average. Fertility refers to the number of actual children a woman
has.
Reproductive value though is hard to know
so the men must have evolved the abilities to notice traits correlated with
reproductive value, such as youth, and health.
In many ways men’s long-term mate
preferences are similar to those of women. Like women, men express a desire for
partners who are intelligent, kind, understanding, and healthy (Buss, 2016 b).
Intelligent long-term mates offer a bounty of benefits—skill at solving
mutually relevant problems of survival, adeptness at childrearing, skill in
navigating social hierarchies, and even good genes that can be transmitted to
children. Kind and understanding partners tend to be empathic, good at social
mind reading, and highly cooperative as long-term partners. Men who select
healthy mates benefit from their ability to thrive in adverse circumstances,
remain energetic in accomplishing the tasks of everyday living, and pass on
genes for good health to their children. Also, like women, men look for
partners who share their values and are similar to them in attitudes,
personality, and religious beliefs. These shared qualities maximize cooperation
in long-term mateships and minimize conflict.
Standards of Beauty Emerge
Early in Life
Adults evaluated color slides of White and
Black female faces for their attractiveness. Then infants 2 to 3 months and 6
to 8 months old were shown pairs of these faces that differed in degree of
attractiveness. Both younger and older infants gazed longer at the more
attractive faces, suggesting that standards of beauty apparently emerge quite
early in life. In a second study, 12-month-old infants played significantly
longer with facially attractive dolls than with unattractive dolls. This
evidence challenges the commonly held view that the standards of attractiveness
are learned through gradual exposure to current cultural models. No training
seems necessary for these standards to emerge.
Standards of Beauty
Are Consistent Across Cultures
The constituents of beauty are neither
arbitrary nor culture bound. When psychologist Michael Cunningham asked people
of different races to judge the facial attractiveness of Asian, Hispanic,
Black, and White women in photographs, he found tremendous consensus about who
is and who is not considered good-looking (Cunningham, Roberts, Wu, Barbee,
& Druen, 1995). The average correlation between groups in their ratings of
the attractiveness of these photographs was +.93. In a second study by the same
investigators, Taiwanese subjects agreed with the other groups in the average
ratings of attractiveness (r = +.91).
Degree of exposure to Western media did not affect the judgments of
attractiveness in either study. In a third study, Blacks and Whites showed
tremendous agreement about which women’s faces were most and least attractive (r = +.94). Consensus has also been found
among Chinese, Indian, and English subjects; between South Africans and North
Americans; and between Russians, Ache Indians, and Americans
So, standards of beauty seem to emerge
early in life and are to some extent consistent across cultures. Why would this
be? In some domains, ancestral men confronted a different set of adaptive
mating problems than did ancestral women, and so their descendants are
predicted to hold a somewhat different set of mate preferences as adaptive
solutions. These preferences start with one of the most powerful cues to a
woman’s reproductive status—her age.
Preference for Youth
Youth is a critical cue because a woman’s
reproductive value declines steadily as she moves past age 20. By the age of
40, a woman’s reproductive capacity is low, and by 50, it is essentially zero.
Men’s preferences capitalize on this. Within the United States, men uniformly
express a desire for mates who are younger than they are. Men’s preference for
youthful partners is not limited to Western cultures.
Our ancestors had access to two types of
observable evidence of a woman’s reproductive value: (1) features of physical appearance, such as full lips, clear
skin, smooth skin, clear eyes, lustrous hair, good muscle tone, and body fat
distribution; and (2) features of behavior,
such as a bouncy, youthful gait, an animated facial expression, and a high
energy level. These physical cues to youth and health, and hence to fertility
and reproductive value, have been hypothesized to be some of the key components
of male standards of female beauty (Symons, 1979, 1995). Strong empirical
support exists for the link between physical attractiveness and objective
measures of health in a sample of roughly 15,000 Americans between the ages of
25 and 34 (Nedelec & Beaver, 2014).
Psychologists Clelland Ford
and Frank Beach discovered several universal cues that correspond with the
evolutionary theory of beauty (1951). Signs of youth, such as clear, smooth
skin, and signs of health, such as an absence of sores and lesions, are
universally regarded as attractive. Cues to ill health and older age are less
attractive. Poor complexion is always considered unattractive. Ringworm, facial
disfigurement, and filthiness are universally undesirable. Even a super-white
sclera, the whites of the eyes surrounding the iris, is key cue to health and
evaluated as attractive (Provine, Cabrera, & Nave-Blodgett, 2013). Freedom
from disease is universally attractive.
Among the Trobriand Islanders in
northwestern Melanesia, for example, anthropologist Bronislaw Malinowski reports
that “sores, ulcers, and skin eruptions are naturally held to be specially
repulsive from the viewpoint of erotic contact” (Malinowski, 1929, p. 244). The
“essential conditions” for beauty, in contrast, are “health, strong growth of
hair, sound teeth, and smooth skin.” Specific features, such as bright, shining
eyes and full, well-shaped lips rather than thin or pinched lips, are
especially important to the islanders.
Another cue to youth and health is
the length and quality of women’s hair.
One study interviewed 230 women at various public locations about their age,
subjective health status, and relationship status and obtained observer
measures of hair length and hair quality (Hinsz, Matz, & Patience, 2001).
Hair length and quality were strong cues to youth: Younger women had longer
hair of higher-rated quality than did older women. Hair quality was positively
correlated with women’s subjective judgments of their own health.
Skin quality is especially important in judgments
of attractiveness. It provides a cue to a woman’s age and a partial record of
her lifetime health (Sugiyama, 2005). Clear, unblemished skin signals an
absence of parasites, absence of skin-damaging diseases during development, and
possibly “good genes” to withstand disease and heal without infection (Singh
& Bronstad, 1997). Skin quality is linked with perceived facial
attractiveness (Fink & Neave, 2005). Female faces with skin that has a
homogeneous skin color distribution, not splotchy, receive higher
attractiveness ratings and are perceived to be younger (Fink, Grammer, &
Matts, 2006; Fink et al., 2008). Furthermore, more skin blood color in female
faces enhances the perception perhaps corresponding to the subjective
impression that some faces seem to “glow” (Stephen, Coetzee, Smith, &
Perrett, 2009). This may also explain why some women use rouge as makeup, since
it enhances perceptions of health and vitality.
Femininity is another cue to attractiveness (Gangestad & Scheyd, 2005).
Facial femininity includes cues such as full lips, relatively large eyes,
thinner jaws, small chin, high cheekbones, and a relatively short distance
between mouth and jaw. Female facial femininity is likely to be a marker of
reproductive value for two reasons. First, as women age, their facial features
become less feminine. Second, facial femininity is linked with higher levels of
estrogen, the ovarian hormone that correlates with fertility (Schaefer et al.,
2006). Third, facial femininity is linked to health and some aspects of disease
resistance (Gray & Boothroyd, 2012). Meta-analyses reveal that facial
femininity is one of the most powerful cues to women’s attractiveness (Rhodes,
2006). Feminine voices—relatively high pitched—are also found to be more
attractive in women and provide cues to youth (Collins & Missing, 2003;
Feinberg et al., 2005; Röder, Fink, & Jones, 2013). Another study using
point-light methodology to measure biomechanical gait found that women who wear
high heels are judged to be both more feminine (shorter stride length and
increased rotation and tilt of the hips) and more attractive (Morris, White,
Morrison, & Fisher, 2013).
Facial symmetry is another correlate of female attractiveness
(Gangestad & Scheyd, 2005; Rhodes, 2006). Symmetry is hypothesized to be a
cue to developmental stability, a hypothesized sign of “good genes” and the
capacity to withstand environmental insult. Symmetrical female faces are indeed
judged to be healthier than less symmetrical faces (Fink et al., 2006). Facial
symmetry is positively correlated with judgments of attractiveness, although
the link is weaker than that of facial femininity (Rhodes, 2006).
Facial averageness is another quality linked with
attractiveness, although this may seem counterintuitive. Researchers created
computer composites of the human face, superimposing faces on each other to
create new faces (Langlois & Roggman, 1990). The new faces differed in the
number of individual faces that made them up—4, 8, 16, or 32. The composite
faces—the averages of the individual faces—were judged more attractive than the
individual faces. And the more faces that went into the composite, the more
attractive the face was judged to be. Two competing hypotheses have been
advanced to explain why average faces are attractive. First, people may show a
generalized cognitive preference for things that are easily processed, and
stimuli that match an average prototype may be easier to process. People do
indeed find averaged images of fish, birds, and even cars more attractive than
individual fish, birds, or cars (Rhodes, 2006). Second, averageness may be a
marker of genetic or phenotypic quality (Gangestad & Scheyd, 2005).
Deviations from averageness may be cues to environmental insults such as such as disease, susceptibility to
environmental insults, or genetic mutations.
Leg length, especially long legs relative to torso length, has been hypothesized
to be a cue to health and biomechanical efficiency (Sorokowski & Pawlowski,
2008). Using silhouette stimuli that held overall height constant but varied
leg length, researchers discovered that legs roughly 5 percent longer than
average are viewed as maximally attractive in women (Sorokowski &
Pawlowski, 2008). Other studies confirm that both sexes view relatively longer
legs as more attractive in women (Bertamini & Bennett, 2009; Swami, Einon,
& Furnham, 2006). Perhaps this explains why some women wear high-heeled
shoes—they make legs appear to be relatively longer. Interestingly, a study of
9,998 Chinese found that women with longer legs had more offspring, an
association especially strong in women from lower socioeconomic backgrounds
(Fielding et al., 2008).
Lumbar curvature. Women face a critical adaptive problem no man has
ever faced—a 9-month pregnancy. As the pregnancy progresses, it shifts women’s
center of gravity forward, changing the strain or torque it puts on their
spinal columns. Women have evolved somewhat different spinal structures—a
wedge-shaped third-to-the last lumbar vertebra—that helps to solve this
adaptive problem. David Lewis and his colleagues calculated the optimal degree
of lumbar curvature that would minimize torque and predicted that men would
find this optimum maximally attractive (Lewis, Russell, Al-Shawaf, & Buss,
2015). Two studies confirmed this hypothesis, highlighting a new discovery
about evolved standards of beauty, guided by an evolutionary perspective.
Reward circuitry in the Brain
Neuropsychologists looked at fMRI images of male
brains as they looked at attractive male, average male, attractive female, and
average female pictures. The attractive female pictures were the ones that
activated the nucleaus accumbens, which is known to be fundamental rewards
circuitry.
Body Fat,
Waist-to-Hip Ratio, and Body Mass Index
The most culturally variable standard of
beauty seems to be in the preference for a slim versus a plump body build, and
it is linked with the social status that build conveys. In cultures where food
is scarce, such as among the Bushmen of Australia, plumpness signals
wealth, health, and adequate nutrition during development (Rosenblatt, 1974).
In ecologies where food shortages are common, such as in Kenya, Uganda, and certain
parts of Ecuador, men prefer women who are heavier and possess more body fat
(Sugiyama, 2005). Even within cultures, men prefer heavier women during
economic hard times (Pettijohn & Jungeberg, 2004), when hungry (Pettijohn,
Sacco, & Yerkes, 2009), and when they feel poor (Nelson & Morrison,
2005). In cultures where food is relatively abundant, such as the United States
and many Western European countries, the relationship between plumpness and
status is reversed, and the wealthy distinguish themselves by being thin
(Symons, 1979). Thus, although “body-weight preference varies across cultures
and time, it does so in predictable ways” (Sugiyama, 2005, p. 318), suggesting
context-dependent adaptations.
Psychologist Devendra Singh has discovered one
preference for body shape that may be universal: the preference for a
particular ratio between the size of a woman’s waist and the size of her hips
(Singh, 1993; Singh & Young, 1995). Before puberty, boys and girls show
similar fat distributions. At puberty, however, a dramatic change occurs. Men
lose fat from their buttocks and thighs, whereas the release of estrogen in
pubertal girls causes them to deposit fat in the lower trunk, primarily on
their hips and upper thighs. Indeed, the volume of body fat in this region is
40 percent greater for women than for men.
Women with a low WHR are
judged to be more attractive than women with a higher WHR. A relatively low WHR
signals that the woman is young, healthy, and not pregnant.
It may also have to do with overall health status and
fertility.
Singh discovered that WHR is a powerful
part of women’s attractiveness. In a dozen studies conducted by Singh, men
rated the attractiveness of female figures that varied in both WHR and total
amount of fat. Again, men found the average figure more attractive than either
a thin or a fat figure. Regardless of the total amount of fat, however, men
find women with low WHRs the most attractive. Women with a WHR of 0.70 are seen
as more attractive than women with a WHR of 0.80, who in turn are seen as more
attractive than women with a WHR of 0.90. Studies with line drawings and with
computer-generated photographic images produced the same results.
Another hypothesized cue to female body
attractiveness is body mass index (BMI), a measure of overall body fat as
calculated from a person’s weight and height. BMI and WHR are positively
correlated—as WHR increases, so does BMI. One study found that a low BMI was a
better predictor of attractiveness judgments than WHR, and that statistically
controlling for BMI, WHR did not predict attractiveness judgments (Cornelissen,
Tovee, & Bateson, 2009). The authors conclude that although WHR is indeed
an important predictor of attractiveness, this is largely explained by the effect
of total body fat on WHR. Another study using an eye-tracking procedure
reinforced this conclusion, finding that eye fixations clustered around the
waist and breasts but not on the pelvic or hip regions (Cornelissen, Hancock,
Kiviniemi, George, & Tovee, 2009). Other research, in contrast, supports
the primacy of WHR over BMI. A brain imaging study found that male brain reward
centers (especially the nucleus accumbens) were activated in response to naked
female bodies with a low WHR, but were not activated by those with a lower BMI
(Platek & Singh, 2010).
More research is needed to settle the
controversy between Waist to Hip Ratio and BMI.
Sex differences in importance
of physical appearance
Because of the abundance of cues conveyed
by a woman’s physical appearance, and because male standards of beauty have
evolved to correspond to these cues, men place a premium on physical appearance
and attractiveness in their mate preferences. A cross-generational mating study
spanning a 57-year period from 1939 to 1996 in the United States gauged the
value men and women place on different characteristics in a mate (Buss et al.,
2001). The same 18 characteristics were measured at roughly one-decade
intervals to determine how mating preferences have changed over time in the
United States. In all cases, men rated physical attractiveness and good looks
as more important than did women.
This does not mean that the importance people place on
attractiveness is forever fixed. On the contrary, the importance of
attractiveness has increased dramatically in the United States in the 20th
century (Buss et al., 2001). For example, the importance attached to good looks
in a marriage partner on a scale of 0 to 3 increased between 1939 and 1996 from
1.50 to 2.11 for men and from 0.94 to 1.67 for women, showing that mate
preferences can change. Indeed, these changes point to the importance of cultural evolution and the impact of input
from the social environment. The sex difference, however, so far remains
invariant.
These sex differences are not limited to
the United States or even to Western cultures. Regardless of location, habitat,
marriage system, or cultural living arrangement, men in all 37 cultures
included in the study on choosing a mate—from Australians to Zambians—valued
physical appearance in a potential mate more than women
Preferences for Ovulating Women
Men may have a preference in ovulating women, since
this would increase their chances of reproductive success.
When women are ovulating their skin becomes more suffused
with blood, and appears to glow more. The skin tone also becomes lighter.
Ovulating women are touched more often in singles bars. Women’s voices also
increase in pitch, their waist to hip ratio tends to lower, and their faces
appear more attractive, and there are other cues as well.
Solutions to the problem of paternity uncertainty
Apes could guard their females during estrus, but due
to cryptic ovulation this becomes more difficult for human males. They need to
spend time on problems of survival, but don’t want to risk the females being unfaithful
and having to invest in someone else’s offspring.
Thus, marriage may have been something to help ensure
paternity certainty, as if they had more sexual access to the females it is
more likely they would be the ones that make the female pregnant.
Also, the desire for premarital sexual chastity and
post marital sexual fidelity. On the assumption that premarital chastity would
be stable over time, it is a clue to a woman’s proclivity to infidelity. A man
who didn’t select a chaste mate ran the risk of getting a female who would
cuckold him.
Men around the world, and through time tended to value
chastity in a partner more than women. Men also highly value fidelity, and
being unfaithful is the most undesirable trait that men find in a partner.
Also, across cultures sexual infidelity is the biggest overall cause of pain
for men.
Positions of power
Men in positions of power and with resources have an
easier time attractive more attractive and younger women. Also, historically
kings and despots were able to accrue several young and attractive mating
partners.
Testosterone levels
Higher testosterone levels help men compete with
same-sex competitors and in their pursuit of females. However, testosterone compromises
the immune system, so after a male has secured a long-term mate, and if he’s
had children the testosterone levels tend to go down.
The mere presence of an attractive female causes men’s
risk taking behaviour to increase, and a rise in testosterone levels.
When people have higher mating value men tend to
allocate more importance to physical attractiveness, and women tend to allocate
more importance to resources. People of higher value tended to be choosier and
thinking about things like kindness and intelligence, etc. But for those lower
in value, for men some basic level of physical attractiveness, for women some
basic level of resources and status.
Women’s competition tactics with other women strongly
map the preferences of men for a long-term mate so, for example, women derogate
other women’s appearance, and sexual fidelity, and promiscuity.
Summary:
Just as women have preferences in a long-term mate
such as access to resources, social status, physical prowess, and masculinity men
have these as well. Although it appears that both sexes appreciate intelligence
and kindness. Men place a greater premium on physical attractiveness in a mate.
Female apes typically go into estrus that signal their fertility, but women
have cryptic fertility. Therefore, men place high value in traits that signal
fertility which includes things like a lower waist to hip ratio, BMI, a
preference for youth and health, facial averageness, facial symmetry, and
femininity among others. Chastity, and to an even greater extent sexual
fidelity are very important to men in long term relationships. In short, there are notable sex differences
in preferences for a long-term mate between men and women and they have deep
evolutionary roots, in contrast to what we hear about gender equality in modern
Western societies.
Source: Evolutionary Psychology The New Science of the
Mind by David Buss 5th Edition
Comments
Post a Comment